On This Page:
Summary
- The frustration-aggression hypothesis is based on the psychodynamic approach. When people are frustrated, they experience a drive to be aggressive toward the object of their frustration, but this is often impossible or inappropriate, so the source of their aggression is displaced by something or someone else.
- It uses the concepts of catharsis (relieving emotional tension) and displacement (unconscious defense mechanism whereby the mind diverts emotions from their original source to a less threatening, dangerous, or unacceptable one to avoid experiencing anxiety).
- Frustration is a feeling of tension that occurs when our efforts to reach a goal are blocked. According to this theory, proposed by Dollard (1939), frustration often leads to aggression.
Background and assumptions
The frustration-aggression hypothesis states that aggression is a result of frustration. Frustration is any event or stimulus that prevents an individual from attaining a goal and it’s accompanying reinforcement quality (Dollard & Miller, 1939).
When our drive to reach a goal is blocked by external factors, we experience frustration, which, in turn, creates an aggressive drive, and this can lead to aggressive behavior.
When we express this aggression physically, verbally, or by fantasizing, we experience catharsis, and our emotional tension is reduced.
However, our aggression is not always expressed towards the legitimate target because it could be too dangerous and we risk punishment, or because this target is not available so we displace our aggressive response towards a less dangerous target or one who just happens to be present. This is called displacement.
The first to formulate the frustration-aggression hypothesis were the Yale University researchers John Dollard, Leonard Doob, Neal Miller, O. H. Mowrer, and Robert Sears (1939).
The group attempted to account for virtually all of human aggression with a few basic ideas in their book, Frustration and Aggression.
Dollard et al. define frustration as an event instead of an affective state (Breuer and Elson, 2017). John Dollard thought about frustration as an unexpected blockage of a goal that someone anticipated attaining.
This characterization of frustration through observable qualities of events and environmental characteristics allows the objective testing and description of its effects rather than relying on subjective self-reported experiences.
This is an important differentiation because this definition of frustration is also implied by modifications and reformulations of the frustration-aggression hypothesis.
A person who loudly insults an instruction manual after two hours of failure in constructing an IKEA wall closet or A toddler who throws a tantrum after noticing that her favorite toy has been placed out of reach on the kitchen table are everyday examples of the link between frustrating events and aggressive responses (Breuer and Elson, 2017).
Examples
Since the 1990s, several studies have either investigated frustration to explain the possible relationship between playing video games and aggression or set out to test the frustration-aggression hypothesis directly for video games.
In one such study, Breuer et al. (2015) investigated the effects of game outcomes and “trash-talking” in a competitive multiplayer sports video game on aggressive behavior.
The researchers showed that unfavorable outcomes (i.e., losing) increase postgame aggression, while trash-talking by the opponent has no such effect.
Testing the frustration-aggression hypothesis, the researchers found that the effect of losing on aggressive behavior is mediated by negative affect, suggesting that the frustration-aggression hypothesis can be applied to the use of video games (Breuer et al., 2015).
However, frustrations can also arise out of the video game in itself, without the presence of a human cosplayer or opponents. For example, a solo player playing a game where there is a mismatch between the skills of the player and the demands of the game may experience frustration.
Additionally, Berkowitz (1989) hypothesized, albeit controversially, that aggressive cues such as violent media content can be a moderator for the relationship between frustration and aggression.
Whitaker et al. (2013) suggested that frustration can be a motivator for people to engage in violent video games, as they allow the player to act aggressively in a virtual environment.
Causes of Frustration
Goal significance and expectations
Historically, behaviorists in early psychology defined frustration as an event resulting from the termination of reinforcement that had previously maintained a behavior.
For example, if a pigeon who had previously received a pellet every time it pushed a lever suddenly ceases to receive a pellet, it would experience frustration (Breuer and Elson, 2017).
Typically, this seizure in reinforcement results in people showing a sudden and temporary increase in the frequency of the behavior that had previously been reinforced, the so-called extinction burst.
However, the taking away of reinforcement can also lead to new behaviors in an attempt to obtain the reward through trial and error.
Amsel (1962) predicts that frustration occurs when the anticipated reward is reduced, and Hanratty et al. (1972) describe frustration as the withdrawal of an anticipated reinforcer (Breuer and Elson, 2017).
Brown and Farber (1951) identified two requirements for an event to be frustrating by Dollard et al.’s (1939) standard: firstly, that achieving the goal must be important or relevant to the individual, and secondly, that achieving the goal must be perceived as a likely outcome by the individual.
Researchers such as Haner and Brown (1955) have also found that the closer a person is to achieving a goal, the more intense the effects of frustration will be on the subsequent aggressive behavior of the person (Harris, 1974; Breuer and Elson, 2017).
This is known as the goal gradient principle (Thompson and Kolstoe, 1974).
Although the extent to which the frustration interferes with the attainment of a desired outcome matters (Berkowitz, 1989), experiencing frustrations while attempting to reach a goal can actually make it more attractive, intensifying the reaction to a following frustration (Filer, 1952).
Self-determination theory (Ryan and Deci, 2000) thinks of frustration as a thwarting of peoples’ basic psychological needs for relatedness, autonomy, and competence.
According to this theory, the presence of aggression-facilitating cues is neither necessary nor sufficient for aggression to occur (Breuer and Elson, 2017).
Interpersonal causes
Competition between multiple people can also be a cause of frustration (Deutsch, 1949). Berkowitz (1989) noted that “competitive encounters are at least partly frustrating as the contestants block each other’s attempts to reach the disputed goal and threaten each other with a total loss.”
Incompetent or selfish cooperators can also cause frustrations as their detrimental behaviors can prevent individuals from attaining personal achievement or groups from reaching a common goal where successful cooperation is essential.
For example, the Robbers Cave experiment, where two groups of adolescents participated in a series of competitive activities for a group trophy and individual prizes, showed that teammates punished those who inhibited group achievement (Sherif, Harvey, White, Hood, and Sherif, 1961; Breuer and Elson, 2017).
Reformulation of the Frustration-Aggression Hypothesis
Dollard et al.’s original formulation of the frustration-aggression hypothesis has not been without great criticism. In response, Berkowitz (1989) reformulated the frustration-aggression hypothesis in a way that most recent research on the causes and effects of frustration use today.
Berkowitz argued that frustration causes a negative effect, and this negative effect elicits aggression. Others have argued that frustration also has effects on cognition and physiological arousal (Anderson and Bushman, 2002; Breuer and Elson, 2017).
Unlike Dollard et al., who implied that aggression is the exclusive result of frustration (1939), Berkowitz reformulated that insults, anxiety (Hokanson, 1961), unpleasant environmental conditions, and aversive effects and circumstances can cause aggression (Breuer and Elson, 2017).
Berkowitz also calls the response to frustration “aggressive inclinations” instead of aggression or aggressive behavior. These inclinations have both cognitive and affective components. This has the implication that the negative effect that frustration causes may not necessarily lead to observable aggression.
A variety of factors can also mediate aggression, such as an individual’s reappraisal of a situation, strong incentives not to be aggressive or aversive consequences for doing so, or no opportunity to behave aggressively toward the source of the frustration.
In short, Berkowitz (1989) reformulated the frustration-aggression theory so that it is more sophisticated but incorporates causes and consequences that are difficult to observe, making it difficult to falsify predictions derived from it.
For example, in a case where someone is frustrated but does not behave aggressively, it may not be easy to determine whether this was due to the absence of negative affect or because somebody did not act on their aggressive inclinations (Breuer and Elson, 2017).
In addition to reformulating the frustration-aggression hypothesis, Berkowitz (1990) created a cognitive neo-association theory of aggression, and other psychologists, such as Anderson and Bushman (2002), have derived their own theories from the frustration-aggression hypothesis.
Breuer and Elson (2017) imagine the link between frustration and aggression as being a multistep model. After experiencing a frustrating event, the individual takes into account several factors, such as the extent to which the frustration is justified, the desirability of the goal, and the extent to which they expected the frustration.
This may move on to negative affect, after which the individual may depending on their tendency toward aggression, irritability, and emotional stability, develop aggressive inclinations.
Whether or not these aggressive inclinations lead to aggressive behavior depends on factors such as social norms, anonymity, visibility of consequences, and the instrumental value of the aggressive act.
Critical Evaluation
- Although some have argued that the expression of aggression serves as a catharsis, Morlan (1949) argues that the expression of aggression sets up a vicious cycle that leads to further aggression, as aggressive acts rarely occur or exist in isolation and have consequences for future interactions (Breuer and Elson, 2017).
- According to Berkowitz, frustration creates an inclination towards aggression but environmental cues may act as a trigger for aggressive behavior. This argument is used to advocate the concealment of weapons in countries such as the US, where people can carry guns, as this could act as a cue to use them. “The finger pulls the trigger, but the trigger may also be pulling the finger.”
- The frustration-aggression hypothesis does not explain individual differences in the way people react to frustration. Some people may withdraw, whereas others will become extremely physically or verbally abusive.
- Brad and Bushman (2002) found that instead of being cathartic as the hypothesis predicts, venting anger makes people more angry and aggressive.
- It explains reactive aggression, which is a response to a threat or provocation, but does not explain pro-active, instrumental (calculated) aggression, where aggression is used as a means to an end.
- It does not take into account free will and moral values; for example, a pacifist individual is unlikely to resort to aggression when experiencing frustration.
The use of aggression is influenced by various factors which were not predicted by the hypothesis:
- Aggression is more likely if the goal is very close than if the achievement of the goal is less likely.
- Aggression is more likely if its use is likely to remove the obstacle to achieving the goal.
- Aggression is more likely if the frustration is justified (Dill & Anderson, 1995)
References
Amsel, A. (1962). Frustrative nonreward in partial reinforcement and discrimination learning: Some recent history and a theoretical extension. Psychological Review, 69(4), 306–328.
Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual review of psychology, 53(1), 27-51.
Berkowitz, L. (1958). The expression and reduction of hostility. Psychological Bulletin, 55(5), 257.
Berkowitz, L. (1988). Frustrations, appraisals, and aversively stimulated aggression. Aggressive behavior, 14(1), 3-11.
Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: examination and reformulation. Psychological bulletin, 106(1), 59.
Berkowitz, L. (1990). On the formation and regulation of anger and aggression: A cognitive-neoassociationistic analysis. American Psychologist, 45(4), 494.
Bettencourt, B., & Miller, N. (1996). Gender differences in aggression as a function of provocation: a meta-analysis. Psychological bulletin, 119(3), 422.
Breuer, J., Scharkow, M., & Quandt, T. (2015). Sore losers? A reexamination of the frustration–aggression hypothesis for colocated video game play. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 4(2), 126.
Breuer, J., & Elson, M. (2017). Frustration-aggression theory (pp. 1-12). Wiley Blackwell.
Brown, J. S., & Farber, I. E. (1951). Emotions conceptualized as intervening variables—with suggestions toward a theory of frustration. Psychological bulletin, 48(6), 465.
Burnstein, E., & Worchel, P. (1962). Arbitrariness of frustration and its consequences for aggression in a social situation. Journal of Personality.
Buss, A. H. (1963). Physical aggression in relation to different frustrations. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67(1), 1.
Buss, A. H. (1966). Instrumentality of aggression, feedback, and frustration as determinants of physical aggression. Journal of personality and social psychology, 3(2), 153.
Caprara, G. V. (1982). A comparison of the frustration-aggression and emotional susceptibility hypotheses. Aggressive Behavior.
Cohen, A. R. (1955). Social norms, arbitrariness of frustration, and status of the agent of frustration in the frustration-aggression hypothesis. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51(2), 222.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York, NY: Harper Perennial.
da Gloria, J. (1984). Frustration, aggression, and the sense of justice. In A. Mummendey (Ed.), Social
psychology of aggression: From individual behavior to social interaction (pp. 127–141). Berlin, Germany: Springer.
Davitz, J. R. (1952). The effects of previous training on postfrustration behavior. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47(2S), 309.
Deutsch, M. (1949). An experimental study of the effects of co-operation and competition upon group process. Human Relations, 2(3), 199–231. doi:10.1177/001872674900200301
Dill, J. C., & Anderson, C. A. (1995). Effects of frustration justification on hostile aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 21(5), 359-369.
Dollard, J., Miller, N. E., Doob, L. W., Mowrer, O. H., & Sears, R. R. (1939). Frustration and aggression.
Geen, R. G. (1968). Effects of frustration, attack, and prior training in aggressiveness upon aggressive behavior. Journal of personality and social psychology, 9(4), 316.
Filer, R. J. (1952). Frustration, satisfaction, and other factors affecting the attractiveness of goal objects. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47(2), 203.
Grossarth-Maticek, R., Eysenck, H. J., & Vetter, H. (1989). The causes and cures of prejudice: An empirical study of the frustration-aggression hypothesis. Personality and Individual Differences, 10(5), 547-558.
Haner, C. F., & Brown, P. A. (1955). Clarification of the instigation to action concept in the frustration-aggression hypothesis. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51(2), 204.
Hanratty, M. A., O”neal, E., & Sulzer, J. L. (1972). Effect of frustration upon imitation of aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21(1), 30.
Harris, M. B. (1974). Mediators between frustration and aggression in a field experiment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10(6), 561-571.
Hokanson, J. E. (1961). The effects of frustration and anxiety on overt aggression. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62(2), 346.
Ichheiser, G. (1950). Frustration and aggression or frustration and defence: A counter-hypothesis.
Kregarman, J. J., & Worchel, P. (1961). Arbitrariness of frustration and aggression. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63(1), 183.
Kulik, J. A., & Brown, R. (1979). Frustration, attribution of blame, and aggression. Journal of experimental social psychology, 15(2), 183-194.
Miller, S., & Mowrer, D. Dollard. 1941.„The Frustration-Aggression Hypothesis.“. Psychological review, 48(4), 337.
Morlan, G. K. (1949). A note on the frustration-aggression theories of Dollard and his associates. Psychological review, 56(1), 1.
Pastore, N. (1950). A neglected factor in the frustration-aggression hypothesis: a comment. The Journal of Psychology, 29(2), 271-279.
Pastore, N. (1952). The role of arbitrariness in the frustration-aggression hypothesis. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47(3), 728.
Rothaus, P., & Worchel, P. (1960). The inhibition of aggression under non-arbitrary frustration. Journal of Personality.
Rule, B. G., Dyck, R., & Nesdale, A. R. (1978). Arbitrariness of frustration: Inhibition or instigation effects on aggression. European Journal of Social Psychology, 8(2), 237-244.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco, CA:Freeman.
Sherif, M., Harvey, O. J., White, B. J., Hood, W. R., & Sherif, C. W. (1961). Intergroup cooperation and competition: The Robbers Cave experiment. Norman, OK: University Book Exchange.
Thompson Jr, R. J., & Kolstoe, R. H. (1974). Physical aggression as a function of strength of frustration and instrumentality of aggression. Journal of Research in Personality, 7(4), 314-323.
Verona, E., & Curtin, J. J. (2006). Gender differences in the negative affective priming of aggressive behavior. Emotion, 6(1), 115.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Whitaker, J. L., Melzer, A., Steffgen, G., & Bushman, B. J. (2013). The allure of the forbidden: Breaking taboos, frustration, and attraction to violent video games. Psychological science, 24(4), 507-513.
Worchel, S. (1974). The effect of three types of arbitrary thwarting on the instigation to aggression 1. Journal of Personality, 42(2), 300-318.
Zillmann, D., & Cantor, J. R. (1976). Effect of timing of information about mitigating circumstances on emotional responses to provocation and retaliatory behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12(1), 38-55.